CS 184: COMPUTER GRAPHICS
PREVIOUS
< - - - - > CS
184 HOME < - - - - > CURRENT
< - - - - > NEXT
Lecture #27 -- Mon 5/04/2009.
Warm-up Problem:

Color Spaces
There are many useful color spaces based on physics, the
human eye, the needs of computer graphics, the way artists'
paints
mix ...
1D Color Space:
Artists color
wheel: "Rainbow colors": -Red-Orange-Yellow-Green-Blue-Purple-
What is red and green together ? ==> (dirty brown).
Cyan, Yellow, Magenta. ==> Subtractive mixing (filters, paint mixing).
For color printers add a good black ==> CYMK system
Computer Scientist’s wheel: 2D: Red, Green, Blue. ==> Additive mixing
(light beams, CRT).
We are missing some colors: where is brown, olive, pink, dark blue ...
?
Color space is 3-dimensional to accommodate also brightness/saturation
variations in addition to hue:
Graphics hardware view: RGB cube, store 3 intensity values for
display on CRT.
Extension of the additive color wheel to a cone (6-sided pyramid)
==> HSV.
Alternative: HLS: Double hex-cone. White/black at tips;
saturated colors on rim at height 0.5.
Physicist’s View: continuous spectrum, infinite dimensions.
Typical spectrum has some broad bumps plus some sharp spectral lines
(added or subtracted). Example: Quasar-Spectrum
Perceptual Colors
The human visual system has three types of cones. (Can only take 3 samples,
each over relatively broad color bands).
Sensitivity of these cones differs: green most sensitive, blue least sensitive.
Metamers: Colors that look the same (P), but have different spectrum(L1,
L2).
Comparative measurements are done with a color-matching set-up:
3 superposed lights (A,B,C) are compared with test color (T).
You can match almost all colors, as far as human perception
goes with an additive mix of the 3 base colors.
The remainder can be matched when one of the 3 colors is placed on
the test light -- to produce a subtractive effect.
This results in color matching functions with positive and negative
coefficients.
==> Perceptual
space is 3-dimensional. (1st Grassman Law )
Grassman’s Laws (of color mixing):
(Grassman measured RGB coordinates of all perceptual colors in 5nm steps through visual spectrum,
1931)
Law #1. Perceptual space is 3-dimensional.
(see above) A basis of three primary colors is sufficent to generate all colors visble to humans.
Law #2. Metamer mix (add) to yield metamers:
L1, L1’ -> P1; L2, L2’ -> P2; then for any a, b :
a*L1 + b*L2 -> P3; then a*L1’ + b*L2’-> P3.
(This is not true for paints or pigments with non-additive behavior; see example
with filter (dashed) below).

Here filter makes no difference -- but here it reduces intensity dramatically!
Law #3. As physical color is varied continuously, perceptual color also
varies continuously.
(Continuity of the perceptual process).
The human retina has three kinds of color sensors (i.e., cones) with peak sensitivities
near 440nm (blue), 545nm (yellow-green), and 580nm (orange) in the
visual part of the spectrum.
The blue sensor is much less responsive than the other two (Foley Fig.13.18).
(There are also "rods" that are more sensitive, but are essentially
black&white sensors).
This leads to the tri-stimulus theory of color perception: which
postulates that colors can be synthesized
from weighted sums of some primary colors, typically in the red, green,
and blue range.
However this is not possible with only positive weighting factors;
some blue-green components of the spectrum require a negatively weighted
red component (Foley Fig.13.20).
This means that the desired color sample has to be changed (de-saturated) with
some red light,
so that it can then be matched by positive blue and green components.
Frequency sensitivity of the eye varies from about 2nm for yellow and
blue to about 10nm near the edge of the visible spectrum.
Overall, about 128 fully saturated hues can be distinguished.
A perceptual
color space was formally defined in 1931 by the Commission International
de l’Eclairage (CIE).
To avoid the need for negative weights, they defined a new set of basis
vectors (primary colors) X, Y, Z,
that lie completely outside the range of all visible RGB values,
so that the color matching functions x(l),
y(l), z(l) for any
visible hue become entirely positive (Foley Fig.13.22).
In addition, the y(l) function is
defined so that it matches the luminosity-efficiency of the human eye (Foley
Fig.13.19);
thus it can be used by itself as an intensity channel.
The transformation from the original RGB color matching functions to
the CIE color matching functions is linear.
If we intersect the visible portion of the CIE color space with a plane
X+Y+Z=1 (about equal brightness),
then we get the horseshoe-shaped region depicted in the CIE
chromaticity diagram.
The fully saturated, pure, spectral colors all lie on the curved part
of the horseshoe.
Somewhere near the center is a point defined as "white".
Colors lying opposite this "white" point are called complementary
colors (Foley Fig.13.26).
A particular tri-stimulus
(3-color) display system has three primaries: I,J,K (Foley Fig.13.27).
It can display only the colors that lie within its convex hull
(=triangle). (Can only use linear combinations of the 3 base colors
with coefficients [0,1]).
Thus we would like to make this triangle as large as possible (-> "ideal
basis"),
but we are limited by what colors of phosphor (or LED's, or gas-discharge)
are available.
The typical CRT display can cover less than half of the visible domain
of colors.
Typical Exam Question:
3D
Color Spaces
Learning by Doing:
Additive
Mixing
Subtractive
Mixing
Metamers
Color
Mixing/Filtering Combination
and
many more ...
(More in Discussion Section)
Reading Assignments: Whatever gives you the needed background information for your project.
Second intermediate report (="AS#10"), Due: FRIDAY, May 8, 2009.
Explain briefly (1 - 5 sentences) what you have constructed so far for your project,
and upload an image of a relevant piece of geometry created for your project.
Put this image, as well as the project description from last, week on your website.
PREVIOUS
< - - - - > CS
184 HOME < - - - - > CURRENT
< - - - - > NEXT
Page Editor: Carlo
H. Séquin