On the Caudal Extension

 

 

Background

 

            Recording from auditory afferent axons from the ear of the American bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana),  several investigators, beginning with Larry Frishkopf and Moise Goldstein, found that the auditory responsiveness  in each axon was sharply tuned, with a best excitatory frequency  (BEF) somewhere between approximately 100 Hz and 1500 Hz (Frishkopf and Goldstein, 1963; Frishkopf and Geisler, 1966; Liff et al., 1968).  Based on their locations in the VIIIth nerve, axons with BEFs below 1000 Hz were presumed to originate at the AP.  Those with BEFs above 1000 Hz were presumed to originate at the basilar papilla (BP).  Later, in the Capranica Lab, Albert Feng, Peter Narins and Bob Capranica  identified two classes of afferent axon from the AP itself (AS Feng, PM Narins & RR Capranica, J. Comp. Physiol. 100: 221-229).  One class, with BEFs from approximately 100 Hz to approximately 700 Hz, exhibited two-tone suppression.  The other class, with BEFs from approximately 400 Hz to approximately 900 Hz did not exhibit that phenomenon. 

 

Bob Capranica and Anne Moffat looked for this other class (higher-frequency axons, without two-tone suppression) in  other anuran species.  In their earliest studies, they found none in Couch’s spadefoot toad (Scaphiopus couchi) (RR Capranica & AJM Moffat, J. Comp. Physiol. 100: 231-245, 1975).  In the green toad (Bufo debilis) they found that a small proportion of the AP afferent axons belonged to this class, and in the American toad (B. americanus) a larger proportion (personal communication).  The proportion in R. catesbeiana had been even larger.  I decided to examine the AP morphology of the four species to determine if there were any obvious differences that might be related to the Capranica-Lab observations. 

 

There were.  R. catesbeiana, B. debilis and B. americanus all exhibited caudal extensions.  S. Couchi did not.  The caudal extension had two features: (1) it was formed by recurving of the AP’s caudal patch, and (2) its hair bundles were oriented in a way that would make them most sensitive to transverse shear of the tectorial membrane (shear normal to the long axis of the patch).  In the rest of the caudal patch, the hair bundles were oriented in a way that  would make them most sensitive to longitudinal shear (shear parallel to the long axis of the patch).  The transition took place under the tectorial curtain.    The proportions of hair cells in the caudal extensions of these three species were good matches to the proportions of higher-frequency afferent axons lacking two-tone suppression (ER Lewis, Scanning Electron Microscopy 1977(II): 429-436, 1977).

 

These results strongly suggested that the AP is tonotopically organized, with the higher-frequency axons originating in the caudal extension.  They also suggested that two-tone suppression is associated with longitudinal motion of the tectorium, and that the mechanical mode of tectorial vibration (in response to sound stimuli) was different on the two sides of the tectorial curtain.   

 

The results encouraged me to expand the comparative morphological studies to other anuran species.  They also added to the urgency of beginning physiological studies of the bullfrog ear in the Lewis Lab—with goal of obtaining functional overlays for the morphological maps we now had for that ear. 

 

In the years that followed, Lewis-Lab studies of bullfrog AP physiology were carried out by Ellen Leverenz, Xiaolong Yu and Walter Yamada.  Andrea Megella Simmons collaborated on part of Ellen’s work (related to adaptation).   Obtaining the functional overlays was achieved by a team that included Ellen and Andrea (see the large section “The Bullfrog Sacculus” in this website).   

 

 

AP tonotopy was corroborated in the American bullfrog. 

 

Based on the observed tonotopy and the evidence from the Capranica Lab, it seems clear that the caudal extension provides at least two things:

 

(1) extension of the range of AP frequency sensitivity—to higher frequencies,

 

(2) a new response feature, namely lack of suppression by a second tone.  

 

 

            During the 1980s, the Narins Lab added two important pieces of information.  The  range of best excitatory frequencies among AP units of Bombina bombina, which has no caudal extension, extends to 700 Hz  (Hillery & Narins,  1987); and the range of best excitatory frequencies among AP units of  Eleutherodactylus coqui, which has the largest proportion of caudal-extension hair cells we have seen so far, extends to 1400 Hz.  And that same range applies to both male and female coqui frogs, although female coqui frogs have a conspicuously larger proportion of caudal-extension hair cells.  If we assume that the Bombina bombina AP data show the greatest range achievable without the caudal extension, then

 

(1a) the extension of the AP frequency range in Eleutherodactylus coqui was one octave.  In the bullfrog it was less than ˝ octave (to approximately 1 kHz.)

           

 

We are left with some puzzles, however.  What selective advantages were provided by the conspicuously greater proportion of hair cells in the female coqui frog? Furthermore, in the bullfrog, the Lewis-Lab dye-tracing studies showed that caudal extension included many units with best excitatory frequencies (450 Hz to 700 Hz) in the range already available without such an extension.   Was lack of two-tone suppression the only selective advantage of these units? 

 

From the Lewis-Lab dye-tracing studies, we have two additional pieces of information for the bullfrog.  The typical afferent axon of the caudal extension innervates one to three hair cells.  The typical afferent axon of the rostral patch innervates six to fifteen hair cells.   This implies that the neural image transmitted to the brainstem from the AP caudal extension has much greater resolution than that from the AP rostral patch.  In Lewis, Hecht & Narins, 1992, we elaborated on this feature and its selective advantages.  The second piece of information was provided by the work of Ellen and Andrea-- weakly and moderately adapting AP afferent axons innervate many hair cells; strongly adapting AP afferent axons innervate only one or a few hair cells.     

 

   Based on these results, it is clear that the caudal extension provides at least two additional things:

 

(3) greatly increased resolution (far more afferent axons per octave) in the spectrographic images sent to the brainstem,

 

(4) greatly increased proportion of a specific response feature, namely strong adaptation.  

 

 

Both of these things should aid the frog in the task of discriminating acoustic signals from one another and from background noise.  

 

For further discussion of this topic, with references to examples of enhanced discrimination, see Lewis, Hecht & Narins, 1992.